Header Ads

PHONOLOGICAL DESCRIPTION OF VOWEL HARMONY IN TÈÈ ON LITERATURE REVIEW


LITERATURE REVIEW
2.0            Introduction
This chapter presents the review of related and relevant literature and is divided into empirical and theoretical review.

2.1     Conceptual Review
          Various approaches have been employed on the discussion of the process of vowel harmony in human languages. Theses include; the segmental approach which views vowels harmony as an assimilation process. The next is linear generative approach which employs the iterative and simultaneous rules to account for vowel harmony and the prosodic approach. According to Katamba (1989:211) vowel harmony is a phonological pattern in which vowels, within some domain-typical the word-share one or more phonological features, like lip rounding or tongue position.
          According to Wikipedia free encyclopedia, vowel harmony is a type of long distance, assimilation phonological process involving the vowels that occur in some language. In languages with vowel harmony, there are constraints on which vowels may be found near each other. In literature, the term vowel harmony is used in two different senses. In the first sense, it refers to any type of long distance assimilatory process of vowels, either progressive or regressive.
          A chapter in West African Linguistics Society (WALS) on vowel quality inventories on vowel harmony described in this way; when a language is said to have vowel harmony this generally means their within a word, including any affixes, it is only possible to combine the members of certain subsets of the vowels together.
          The chapter continues the vowel that causes the vowel assimilation is frequently termed the trigger while the vowels that assimilate (or) harmonized are termed targets in most languages, the vowel triggers lie within the root of a word while the affixes add3ed to the roots contain the targets. This may be seen in the Hungarian dative suffix below.
          Root           Dative                  Gloss
          Város                   Varos-nak   ‘city’
          Öröm          öröm-nek    ‘Joy’

The dative suffix has two different forms-nak-nek. The form appears after the root with back vowels (a and o are both back vowels). The -nek form appears after the root with front vowels (ö and e are front vowels).
          Questions about how vowel harmony starts in languages have not reviewed clear-cut answers. Probably, the most common explanation of how vowel harmony starts in that it is a grammaticalization of the phonetic effect of co-articulation, where the properties of one segment influence how the speaker articulates surrounding segments. However, phonologists in recent times have come to terms to certain features which include:
(i)                Vowel height (i.e. high, mid or low vowels) vowel backwardness (i.e. front, central or back vowels).
(ii)             Vowel roundness (i.e. rounded or unrounded).
(iii)           Tongue root position (i.e. advance or retracted tongue root, abbreviated + ATR).
(iv)           Nasalization (i.e. oral or nasal) in this case, a nasal consonant is usually the trigger).
2.2     Languages with Vowel Harmony
          Specifically, languages with clear cut examples of vowel harmony are mainly, African languages. In recent times, evidences from literature shows that some Turkic and Uralic languages have been linked to vowel harmony. Below are some of the language with vowel harmony apart from those of African languages.

2.2.1  Korean
          According to Kramer, Martin (2003), there are three classes of vowels in Korean: positive, negative, and neutral. These categories loosely follow the front (positive) and mid (negative) vowels. Traditionally, Korean had strong vowel harmony; however, this rule is no longer observed strictly in modern Korean. For Vago, Robert M. (1994). In modern Korean, it is only applied in certain cases such as onomatopoeia, adjectives, adverbs, conjugation, and interjections. The vowel (eu) is considered a partially neutral and a partially negative vowel. There are other traces of vowel harmony in modern Korean: many native Korean words tend to follow vowel harmony such as (saram).

2.2.2  Mongolian
          According to Kramer, Martin (2003) Mongolian possesses a different system of vowel harmony. The system includes both a pharyngeal harmony and a rounding harmony. In particular, the pharyngeal harmony involves the vowels; a, U, O (pharyngeal) and i, u, e, o (non-pharyngeal). Rounding harmony only affects the open vowels, e, o, a.
                    ö       e       a
                    ö                o
                    y       i        u
For Vago, Robert M. (1994), Finish vowel harmony and case agreement exemplified by mahdollisisa yllättävissää tilanteissa (“in possible unexpected situations”): mahdollinen takes -ssa, yllättävä takes saa and yllättävä, with a neutral vowel first but a back vowel second, takes -ssä.
          In the Finish Language, there are three classes of vowels – front, back, and neutral, where each front vowel has a back vowel pairing. Grammatical endings such as case and derivational endings – but not enclitics – have only archiphonemic vowels U, O, A, which are realized as either back [u, o, a] or front [y, ø, æ] inside a single word. From vowel harmony it follows that the initial syllabus of each single (non-compound) word controls the frontness or backness of the entire word. Non-initially, the neutral vowels are transparent to and unaffected by vowel harmony. In the initial syllabus.
1.                 A back vowel causes all non-initial syllables to be realize with back (or neutral) vowels, e.g. pos+ahata+(t)a →
2.                 A front vowel causes all non-initial syllables to be realized with front (or neutral) vowels, e.g. räj+ahta+(t)a→räjähtää.
3.                 A neutral vowel acts like a front vowels, but does not control the frontness or backness of the word: if there are back vowels in non-initial syllables, the word acts like it began with back vowels, even if they come from derivational endings, e.g. sih+ahta+(t)a → sihahtaa cf. sih+ise+(t)a→sihistä.

For example:
·        Kaura begins with back vowel – kauralla
·        Kuori begins with back vowel – kuorella
·        Sieni begins without back vowels – sienellä (not *sienella)
·        Käyrä begins without back vowels – Käyrällä
·        Tuote begins with back vowels – tuotteeseensa
·        Kerä begins with a neutral vowel, but has a non-initial back vowel – Keralla.

He went on to say that some dialects that have a sound, change opening diphthong codas also permit archiphonemic vowels in the initial syllable. For example, standard i.e. is reflected as ‘ia’ or ‘iä’, controlled by non-initial syllables, in the tampere dialect, e.g. tiä – tie btu miakka – miekka.
          … as evidenced by tuotteeseensa (not *tuotteeseensä). Even if phonologically front vowels precede the suffix-vowel harmony. For example, Olympia is often pronounced lumpia. The position of some loans is unstandardized (e.f. chattailla/chättäillä) or ill standardized (e.g. polymeeri, sometimes pronounced polumeeri, and atoritäärinen, which violate vowel harmony). Where a foreign word violates vowel harmony by not using front vowels because it begins with a neutral vowel, then last syllable generally counts, although this rule is irregularly followed. Experiments indicates that e.g. miljonääri always becomes (front miljonääriä, but marttyyri becomes equally frequently both marttyyria (back) and Marttyyriä (front), even by the same speaker.
          With respect to vowel harmony, compound words can be considered separate words. For example, syyskuu (“autumn month” i.e. September) has both u and y, but it consist of two words syys and kuu, and declines “to back” and – pain “wards”, which give e.g. taakseäinkään (not *taaksepäinkaan or *taaksepainkaan). If fusion takes place, the vowel is harmonized by some speakers, e.g. tälläinen pro tällainen – tämän lainen.
          Some Finnish words whose stems contain only neutral vowels exhibits an alternative pattern in terms of vowel harmony when inflected or forming new words through derivation. Examples includes meri “sea”, meressä “in the sea” (inessive), but merta (partitive), not *merta; very “blood”, verestä “from the blood” (elative).

2.2.3  Hungarian
          Hungarian lies its distant relative Finnish, has the same system of front, back, and intermediate (neutral vowels. The basic rule is that words with front (“high”) vowels get from vowel suffixes (Kèzbe -in (to) the hand), back, (“low”) vowel words back suffixes (Karba -in (to) the arm).
          The only essential difference in classification between Hungarian and Finnish is that most dialects of Hungarian do not observe the difference between Finnish ‘ä’ [æ] and ‘e’ [e] – the Hungarian front vowel ‘e’ [æ] is the same as the Finnish front vowel ‘ä’.

2.2.4  Behaviour of Neutral Vowels
          Intermediate or neutral vowels are usually counted as front ones, since they are formed that way, the difference being that neutral vowels can occur along with back vowels in Hungarian word bases (e.g. rèpa carrot, kocsi car). The basic rule is that words with neutral and back vowels usually take back suffixes (e.g. rèpa/ban in a carrot, kocsi/ban in a car). The suffix rules for words with both kinds of suffixes are the following:
·        The last syllable counts (typically in words of visibly foreign origin): soför/höz, nUáansz/szal, general/ás, október/ben, parlament/ben, szoftver/rel.
·        A regular exception i/l and è (but usually e): they are transparent for the rule, so only the other sounds will be taken into consideration, e.g. papir/hoz, kuplè/hoz, marèk/hoz, konflis/hoz.
·        Some l-syllable words using l are strictly using front suffixes (vis >/et, hlr/ek), while some others can take back suffixes only (szlj/ról, nyil > nyil/at, zsir/ban, ir/ás).
·        Some words can take either front or back suffixes: farmer/ban or farmer/ben.

Suffixes in multiple forms
          While most grammatical suffixes in Hungarian come in either one form (e.g. -kor) or two forms (front and back, e.g. -ban/-ben), some suffixes have additional form for front rounded vowels (such as ö, ö, Uá and Uá), e.g. hoz/hez/-höz. An example on basic numerals.

2.3     Estonian
          Standard Estonian has lost its vowel harmony, the front vowels occurring only in the first (stressed) syllable. However vowel harmony exists in its võro dialect.

2.4     Types of Harmony
          Although vowel harmony is the most well known harmony, not all types of harmony that occur in the world’s languages involved only vowels. Other types of harmony involved consonants (and is known as consonant harmony. Rarer types of harmony are those that involved tone or both vowels and consonants (e.g. postvelar harmony).

2.4.1  Vowel Consonant Harmony
          Some languages have harmony processes that involved an interaction between vowels and consonants. For example, Chilcotin has a phonological process known as vowel flattening i.e. post-velar harmony) where vowels must harmonize with uvular and pharynegealized consonants.

          “flat” vowels [i, e, i, o, O, «, a]
          Non “flat” vowels [i, i, u, U, æ, E]

The result was that back vowels were fronted after j or a palatal consonant, and consonants were palatalized before j or a front vowel. Diphthongs were harmonized as well, although they were soon monophthongized because of a tendency to end syllables with a vowel (syllables were or became open). This rule remained in place for a long time, and ensured that a syllable containing a front vowel always began with a palatal consonant, and a syllable containing j was always preceded by a palatal consonant and followed by front vowel.

2.5     Empirical Review
N
 
          Generally, few words are available in Tèè linguistically speaking. Some of them include, Waboso (1995), worked on the consonant system of Tèè and concluded that the language has 31 phonetic consonants and 21 phonemic consonants. Nlem (1995) worked on syllable, morpheme and word structure of Tèè. He discovered that the languages has marginal syllable structure with (C) V (C) as the Syllable schema.



2.5.1  Theoretical Framework
          The theoretical framework adopted for this study is auto-segmental. The most fundamental characteristics of the autosegmental theory is that, phonological representation is non-linear, that is, that a phonological representation is composed not of a single sequence of entity but of several parallel sequences of entities arranged in two or more tiers each of which is said to be independent of the others. They are arranged in separate tiers through the use of association lines.
          According to Goldsmith (199:137), the main focus of this theory is the analysis of tone as an auto-segment capable of having an independent existence from the segment that bears them. Tone is seen as an associated feature of the segment through co-articulation with the vowels.
          Within the standard generative phonology however, phonological representation was conceived as a linear arrangement of segment and boundaries consisting of segmental and suprasegmental representations. It was assumed that, superimposed on the segmental layer were tone and stress and possibly a few other phenomena such as vowel harmony. Both segmental and suprasegmental elements were thought to be arranged in a row one after the other (Goldsmith 1990:10, Katramba 1989:189) and Aziza 1997:8).

No comments

Powered by Blogger.